Archives of Personal Papers ex libris Ludwig Benner, Jr.
- - - - - -Last updated on Wed, Aug 8, 2012
Reprinted from the Journal of Safety Research, June 1975/Vol. 7/No. 2 |
Ludwig Benner, Jr.[1]
Difficulties experienced by accident investigators in explaining what happened and why it happened in specific accident investigations are examined. These include problems with delineating the beginning and end of the accident, methods for discovering and testing the relevance of facts of the accident, and methods for presenting the findings of the investigation. Criteria for approaches to resolve these difficulties are suggested. Concepts influencing current methods of research and investigations are discussed, along with their limitations in terms of the suggested criteria. A generalized explanation for the accident phenomenon based on the role of perturbation in an events sequence (P-theory) is proposed, and a charting method for ordering the events of a particular accident is presented. Development of an events indexing system for future accident data collection is suggested. Over the years, accident investigations have been performed for many different reasons by many different persons with varied backgrounds and skills. The results have been equally varied in terms of quality. Regardless of the reason for the investigation, however, each investigator is confronted by certain recurring questions each time he begins an investigation of an accident. The first of these are: What happened? Why did it happen as it did? These questions give rise to still other questions, such as: What should my investigation encompass? What facts do I need? How do Ianalyze the facts I acquire? How can I explain the relationships among the factors? How should I present my findings? Despite the vast number of accidents reported on or researched for understanding, there still exists no generally accepted set of answers to these questions that can be applied to the full range of accident phenomena. Approaches to accident investigation seem as diverse as the investigators, who range from highly educated medical doctors and doctors of philosophy and engineering to the relatively less trained traffic policemen investigating traffic accidents. The absence of a common approach and differences in the investigative and analytical methods used have resulted in serious difficulties in the safety field. These include: (1) Barriers to a common understanding of the phenomenon and to the transfer of knowledge about safety countermeasures gleaned from individual accidents; (2) popular misconceptions about the nature of the accident phenomenon; (3) injustices in the determination of culpability of the persons involved in specific accidents; and (4) inefficiencies in the development of safety countermeasures. The purpose of this paper is to call attention to the need to develop generally acceptable approaches and analysis methods that will result in complete, reproducible, conceptually consistent, and easily communicated explanations of accidents. These four attributes are suggested as the criteria for any approaches eventually adopted in the field. What happened? One of the first steps in any accident investigation is to determine what constitutes the "accident" being investigated. It is rare that any investigator defines the beginning and end of the phenomenon he is setting out to investigate. Consequently, the likelihood of achieving reproducible results is sharply reduced. In a search of over 100 papers on accident investigation and accident research, primarily in the transportation field, no clear delineation of the beginning and end of the accident phenomenon could be found.[2] |
It is assumed that accidents involve the occurrence of a set of successive events that produce unintentional harm. It is further assumed that the events occurred during the conduct of some activity with an expected outcome other than injury or damage (Lawrence, 1974). Additional assumptions emerge when these two are further examined. For example, what is meant by events? Since they constitute the building blocks of the accident, their meaning should be clear so that the investigator describes each event precisely. This is equally true for the other terms employed.
For accident investigation purposes, an event is something that takes place, an occurrence of moment or significance, a happening logically ensuing from or giving rise to another happening. For something to happen, someone or something must bring it about. That someone or something will be termed an actor, and the act of doing something will be termed an action. Thus, an event can be described in terms of an actor and an action. The actor can be anything animate, such as a driver, machinist, or bee or anything inanimate such as a tire, machine, or water. The action may be anything that occurs, such as movement, failure, observation, or decision. Both the actor and the action must be described precisely or quantitatively, without qualitative adjectives, adverbs, or phrases. Events must be described in terms of a single actor and action, which means breaking down the sequence into discrete events that can be so described. It is further assumed that an activity is occurring when an accident begins. An activity is defined as a set of successive events directed -consciously or unconsciously-toward some anticipated or intended outcome. An activity proceeds toward its outcome in a relatively stable progression of events, involving interact- ing elements, in the face of varying external influences. This dynamic equilibrium of successive events progresses in a state of homeostasis (Pask, 1963), requiring adaptive behavior or adaptive learning by the actors involved in maintaining the stable flow of events. The external influences are termed perturbations when they vary or deviate from what is usual or expected, disturbing the activity and initiating the possibility of subsequent injury or damage. As long as the actors adapt to the perturbations encountered without being stressed beyond their capability to adapt or recover (Benner, 1975) homeostasis is maintained and an accident does not occur. If one of the actors fails or is unable to adapt, the perturbation initiates an events sequence that ends homeostasis and begins the accident sequence. From that event onward, the events sequence may overstress one of the actors, causing injury or damage. These injurious events may in turn initiate other changes or energy releases that overstress subsequently exposed actors, with cascading injury or damage. Until the subsequently stressed actors are able to accommodate the impinging stresses without further harm, the accident continues toward its outcome, governed by the condition of the actors, the events, and the laws of nature. Thus, the accident can be seen to begin with a perturbation and end with the last injurious or damaging event in the continuing accidental events sequence. If the actors adapt to the perturbation at any time before injury occurs, a no-accident outcome is achieved, even though the activity may be disordered. At present, this result is generally described as a "near miss. The above explanation provides a basis for defining the beginning and end of any accident being investigated and for developing and ordering the events sequence of the accident. Since it is centered on perturbations and subsequent events, this explanation of accidents will be termed the P-theory of accidents. Stress may be imposed by change (Johnson, 1975) or by impinging energy (Haddon, 1970). Within this framework, the accident phenomenon can be further explained by the use of a charting technique that displays the events in terms of their chronological relationships (Figure 1). Displaying accident events sequences. The events charting method described above suggests an approach for resolving the remaining difficulties confronting the investigator. A chronological array of the events identified helps to structure the search for the relevant factors and events involved in the accident and provides a method for testing the relevancy of additional events or conditions encountered by the investigator. This can be achieved by displaying the events in which each actor is involved along a horizontal line. en Since each actor is usually involved in sequential events, the events for each may be displayed in a linear chain of events. Arrows depicting the flow of the events in a logical sequence can link each entry, thus showing the relationship of events to one another. By adding the conditions that must have existed for the events to occur and linking them to the events with arrows, the full explanation of the events for any actor involved in the accident can be shown. By arraying the events associated with each actor in parallel horizontal lines across the chart, with the timing of each event maintained relative to the other events, one can readily see the relationship of each event to every other event in terms of both its timing and its proceed/follow logic. This multilinear events sequence charting method provides a way for the investigator of any accident to identify, order, and display the explanation of the relevant factors in the accident. When combined with en the generalized explanation of accidents provided by the P-theory and the criteria for investigative decisions arising from that explanation, the tools for answering the questions faced by the investigator are now available. Application of method. Most systems safety charting methods use rectangular boxes to depict events and ovals to depict conditions. Using the arrow convention suggested, Figure 2 shows the events constituting an activity and the outcome of that activity for one actor. When two or more actors produce the outcome, the events are displayed as in Figure 3. Note that the spacing of each event can be used to show the timing of the event in relation to the other events. The arrows provide flexibility because they can be used to show the flow relationships of the events or proceed/follow logic between events. Conditions can be shown similarly (Figure 4). This method for presenting the accident events and enabling conditions is keyed to the time order and logical flow of events present in an accident, rather than just the time order of events suggested by Johnson (1975). The method is summarized in Figure 5. Tracing the development of enabling conditions in an accident can be accomplished using the same approach. By considering such a condition as the outcome of another external influencing activity, the events producing that outcome can be traced backward in time until the needs of the analyst are met. This sequence can be displayed vertically, above the enabling condition. This approach allows the analyst to explore the factors that might be common to more than one accident and locate correctable circumstances, while separating such factors from the specific accident phenomenon being investigated. The general flow of the charting is shown in Figure 6. A simple household accident may be used to illustrate the method. An occupant (actor A) slips on a throw rug (actor B), falls, and gets hurt. Applying the principle of breaking down the accident into the discrete actions of each actor, and using the P-theory, A's activity (walking to change his location) was homeostatic until he stepped on B. For A, the first event (E1a) in this specific accident is the placement of his foot on B, followed by the unexpected motion of his foot (E2a) when the rug begins to slide. For B, the first event (Elb) is the experiencing of directional stress levels (A's foot placement) in excess en of the frictional shear resistance capability of B with respect to the floor, followed by movement of the rug (E2b) from under A's foot. As this is occurring, A loses his balance or fails to adapt (E3a), falls to the floor (E4a), and suffers injury (E5a). The intended activity terminates with the accidental outcome. For the rug to slip, the coefficient of friction had to have been reduced to some value X; this enabling condition (C1b) must have existed for the rug to begin to slide. The rug was not nailed down (C2b), so it slid from under the foot of A. The same approach would be applicable to other events and conditions indicated by the evidence in the accident. The method provides criteria for delineating the events and conditions of the accident and for ordering them according to their logic and relative timing during the accident. Substituting the perturbing, adaptive, stressing, injury, and stress accommodating events in the above display, in whatever detail may be required to fully explain the accident, substantially increases the likelihood of producing a complete, reproducible, conceptually consistent and easily communicated explanation of the accident being investigated. Conclusions. The development of a generalized explanation of the accident phenomenon is possible and would fill a major need of accident investigators. The multilinear events sequence charting method provides criteria to guide the development of the explanation of specific accidents in a manner that facilitates the transfer of knowledge among accident investigators. By incorporating an event indexing system (Johnson, 1975) into an expanded P-theory model, a method for rational acquisition of accident data compatible across accident types might be devised (Wakeland, 1971). These data could then be applied to the development of accident events sequence models for use by accident investigators, by researchers, and by risk analysts attempting to estimate effects of safety countermeasures (NTSB, 1971). An improved public grasp of the accident phenomenon, which could follow the adoption of the P-theory and the charting methods, would help to resolve the other difficulties cited. REFERENCESAmes, J. S. Aircraft accidents-method of analysis. Transactions of the National Safety Council, 17th Annual Congress. Chicago: National Safety Council, 1928. Baker, J. S. Problems of deteRmining causes of specific accidents. Evanston, Ill.: Traffic Institute, Northwestern University, 1953. Baker, J. S.; & Ross, H. L. Concepts and classification of traffic accident causes. Evanston, Ill.: Traffic Institute, Northwestern University, 1960. Benner, L. Safety, risk and regulation. Transportation Research Forum Proceedings, Chicago, 1972,13, 1. Driessen, C. G. J. Cause tree analysis: Measuring how accidents happen and the probabilities of their cause. Paper presented at the 78th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, Miami, September, 1970. Haddon, W. Jr., On the escape of tigers: An ecological note. American Journal of Public Health, 1970, 2229. Heinrich, H. W. Industrial accident prevention. New York: McGraw Hill, 1936. Johnson, W. C. MORT: The management oversight and risk tree. Journal of Safety Research, 1975, 7, 4-15. Lawrence, A. C. Human error as a cause of accidents in gold mining. Journal of Safety Research, 1974, 6, 78-88. Pask, G. Learning behavior, in nerve, brain and memory models. In Progress In Brain Research (Vol.2). Amsterdam: Elsevier Publishing Co., 1963. Surry, J. Industrial accident research. Toronto, Canada: University of Toronto, 1969. US National Transportation Safety Board. Special study, risk concepts in dangerous goods transportation regulations. NTSB STS 71-1, Washington, D.C., 1971. Wakeland, H. H. Personal communication, 1971. [1] Ludwig Benner, Jr., is a General Engineer, National Transportation Safety Board, Washington, D.C. The views expressed are those of the author and not necessarily those of the National Transportation Safety Board. [2] 'A listing of all of the articles searched is available from the author or from the Journal of Safety Research. EpilogueThis paper is one of two* initial reports about over 30 years of inquiries that have contributed to the continually emerging concepts for accident investigation, led to Multilinear Events Sequencing technology for investigations, and introduced a number of innovations. It challenged the dominant accident models and theories drining investigations at the time, and introduced an early process model for accidents; defined the actor/action sturcturing of the building blocks used to describe the dynamics of what happened; introduced a system safety approach for investigations; pointed out that events during accidents occur both in series and in parallel and the significance of the timing those actions; proposed flow charting the entire accident phenomenon to identify, order and display interactions and why they happened, in the sense of what is now known as "defined acyclical graphs"; pointed out the role of adaptative responses to deviations in accident processes and prevention efforts, presaging expanded ideas about system control in recent accident analysis literature; raised the challenge of validating relationships for objective quality assurance (still largely ignored); and suggested that investigations have definable beginning and end attributes. Subsequent to publication of this paper in 1975, observations of applications of this method disclosed a flaw in this approach. Conditions were included to explain the "why" because that was consistent with the conventional wisdom of the time. Unfortunately, investigators using the MES approach were observed to use conditions as a license to introduce ambiguities, judgments, and conclusions into the flowchart. That in turn frustrated the application of rigorous logical linkages to define and explain interactions that happened, and attempts to apply quality assurance procedures to the flow charts. Worse, it masked the action patterns of people or objects that had to be changed to reduce future risks. The method was subsequently refined by limiting the worksheets to displaying what happened as event building blocks, and excluding conditions. Conditions, whether static or dynamic states, are produced by the actions by people or objects or energies. Such actions also lead to subsequent events, all of which were required to produce the accident outcomes - harm, damage and loss. Constraining the flow chart to actions enables investigators to perform progressive analyses as the data are acquired, to direct their investigation efforts to specific data gaps, and to do logic tests to identify, define and act on actions or behaviors which posed problems. These changes were elaborated in more detail in Hendrick and Benner INVESTIGATING ACCIDENTS WITH STEP (1986) and later publications, pamphlets on Accident, Hazmat and Fire investigations published in 1997, MES implementation software published in 2006 and a series of papers analyzing lessons learning systems published since 2007. All are available at www.iprr.org. With respect to the "conditions" ideas presented here, the paper is obsolete. Unfortunately, implementation of the MES methods in many locations, including MORT-based applications, is based on this 1975 version of MES, rather than the latest technology found in subsequent publications. Readers interested in applying the MES technology are urged to refer to subsequent publications available at this site. As underlying assumptions continute to be challenged, substantial changes continue to evolve. * See Accident Theory and Accident Investigation (October 1975) L Benner |